The Contributions of Singular and Plural Nouns to Sentence Processing Complexity: Evidence from Reading Time

Authors

  • Rachel H. Messer Bethel College, KS, USA
  • Shelia Kennison Oklahoma State University, USA

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.29038/eejpl.2020.7.1.mes

Keywords:

reading, semantic representations, syntactic ambiguity, comprehension, plural nouns, singular nouns.

Abstract

The nature of semantic representations of plural nouns has been a subject of debates in the literature. The present research investigated the extent to which there are differences in the processing of plural versus single noun descriptions (e.g., the large chairs vs. the large chair).  In two reading experiments, we tested whether plural (versus singular) nouns appearing in sentences were more difficult to process initially and/or led to increased processing difficulty when occurring in sentences that contain a temporary syntactic ambiguity. Reading time on syntactically ambiguous sentences containing plural or singular nouns were compared with reading time on unambiguous control sentences. The results of both experiments demonstrated significant effects of sentence ambiguity.  No effects or interactions involving noun number were observed, indicating that the complexity of plural nouns does not result in processing difficulty during sentence comprehension.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biographies

  • Rachel H. Messer, Bethel College, KS, USA

    Scopus Author ID: 56957958400

    ORCID: 0000-0002-0272-6126

    rmesser@bethelks.edu

  • Shelia Kennison, Oklahoma State University, USA

    Scopus Author ID: 6603400736

    ORCID: 0000-0001-9298-3152

    shelia.kennison@okstate.edu

References

Adams, B., Clifton, C., & Mitchell, D. (1998). Lexical guidance in sentence processing? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5(2), 265–270. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212949

Baayen, R. H., Dijkstra, T., & Schreuder, R. (1997). Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 94–117. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2509

Barker, C. (1992). Group terms in English: Representing groups as atoms. Journal of Semantics 9(1), 69–93. https://doi.org/10.1093/jos/9.1.69.

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 22, 577–660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149.

Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 12, 335–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80014-3

Dominguez, A., Cuetos, F., & Segui, J. (1999). The processing of grammatical gender and number in Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(5), 485–498. https://doi.org/10.1023/ A:1023216326448

Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. (1990). Use of verb information during syntactic parsing: Evidence from eye tracking and word by word self-paced reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 16, 555–568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.555

Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1991). Recovery from misanalyses of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(6), 725–745. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749- 596X(91)90034-H.

Ferreira, F., & McClure, K. K. (1997). Parsing of garden-path sentences with reciprocal verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 273–306. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909697386862

Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory & Language, 37, 58–93. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2512

Johnson-Laird, P. (1983). Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inference, and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kennison, S. M. (2001). Limitations on the use of verb information in sentence comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 132–138. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196149

Kennison, S. M. (2005). Different time courses of integrative semantic processing for plural and singular nouns: Implications for theories of sentence processing. Cognition, 97, 269–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.09.005

Mitchell, D. C. (1987). Lexical guidance in human parsing: Locus and processing characteristics. In M. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and Performance 12: The Psychology of Reading (pp. 601–618). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

New, B., Brysbaert, M., Segui, J., Ferrand, L., & Rastle, K. (2004). The processing of singular and plural nouns in French and English. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 568–585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.010

Patson, N. D. (2014). The processing of plural expressions. Language and Linguistics Compass, 8(8), 319–329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12085

Patson, N. D., George, G., & Warren, T. (2014). The conceptual representation of number. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(7), 1349–1365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.863372

Patson, N.(2014). The processing of plural expressions. Language and Linguistics Compass, 8(8), 319–329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12085

Patson, N. (2016). Evidence in support of a scalar implicature account of plurality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(7), 1140–1153. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000224

Patson, N. D., & Ferreira, F. (2009). Conceptual plural information is used to guide early parsing decisions: Evidence from garden-path sentences with reciprocal verbs. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 464–486.

Patson, N., George, G., & Warren, T. (2014). The conceptual representation of number. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(7), 1349–1365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.863372

Patson, N., & Warren, T. (2011). Building complex reference objects from dual sets. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 443–459. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.863372

Patson, N., & Warren, T. (2014). Comparing the roles of referents and event structures in parsing preferences. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 408–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.788197

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime (Version 2.0). [Computer software and manual]. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

Schreuder, R., & Baayen, R. (1997). How complex simple words can be. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 118–139. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2510

Schwarzschild, R. (1996). Pluralities. Kluwer, Dordrecht.

Sereno, J. A., & Jongman, A. (1997). Processing of English inflectional morphology. Memory &

Cognition, 25(4), 425–437. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201119

Sturt, P., Pickering, M. J., & Crocker, M. W. (2000). Search strategies in syntactic reanalysis. Journal

of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(2), 183–194. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005145028351 Zwaan, R. A., Stanfield, R. A., & Yaxley, R. H. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological science, 13(2), 168–171. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-

00430

Downloads

Published

2020-06-30

Issue

Section

Vol 7 No 1 (2020)

How to Cite

Messer, R. ., & Kennison, S. (2020). The Contributions of Singular and Plural Nouns to Sentence Processing Complexity: Evidence from Reading Time. East European Journal of Psycholinguistics , 7(1), 85-96. https://doi.org/10.29038/eejpl.2020.7.1.mes

Most read articles by the same author(s)