Does ‘Language’ Form Our ‘Thought’?

Authors

  • Rieko Matsuoka National College of Nursing, Japan

Keywords:

linguistic relativity, zero personal pronouns, low-context society, high-context society, seken (life-world), Japanese sense of self.

Abstract

As early as 1799, Humboldt initiated to wonder the gravity of ‘language’. Indeed, there exists much diversity in linguistic forms in human societies and, translation is necessary in order to share literary works, among different language users. During the process of translating culturally-colored discourse, some important features of a given society may well be revealed. As an empirical example, a script of rakugo, which is the traditional Japanese performance art of telling comic stories, is used as the data for analysis because rakugo can be regarded as a genre of natural, spoken Japanese discourse. In fact, Katz (as cited in Wardy, 2006) suggests that linguistic relativity threatens universal inter-translatability. In this study, focusing on zero personal pronouns, the notion of linguistic relativity is examined, related to the Japanese sense of self and the Japanese worldview that includes seken (life-world). This seems to reveal linguistic relativity (e.g., Humboldt 1999; Sapir 1921/2004) that different perspectives on reality often manifest themselves as specific features of language use in speech communities, as some empirical studies have been conducted, starting with Humboldt’s original research (1999) on the Kawi language.

References

  • Abe, Kinya. (2001). Academia and [seken] (Gakumon to [seken]: 学問と「世間」).
    Tokyo: Iwanami.
  • Adler, G. J. (2009). Wilhelm Von Humboldt’s Linguistical Studies, BiblioBazaar.
  • Bloomfield, L. (1922). Review of language. Classical Weekly, 15, 142-143.
  • Boronditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?: Mandarin and English speakers’
    conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1-22.
  • Chaudhary, N. (2003). Speaking the self into becoming? Culture & Psychology, 9(4), 471-
    486.
  • Davies, I. R. L. (1998). A study of colour grouping in three languages: A test of the
    linguistic relativity hypothesis. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 433-452.
  • Hall, E. (1976). Beyond culture. Garden City, New York: Anchor Press.
  • Handler, R. (1986). The aesthetics of Sapir’s language. In: New perspectives in language,
    culture, and personality, William Lowan, Michael. K. Foster, and E. F. K. Koerner, Eds.
    Amsterdam: John Benjamin.
  • Humboldt, W. von. 1999. On Language: on the diversity of human languages construction
    and its influence on the mental development of the human species. Cambridge University Press.
  •  January, D., Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to
    Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 417-426.
  • Kuwayama, T. (1992). The reference other orientation. In: Japanese sense of self, Nancy
  • Rosenberger, Ed. New York: Cambridge University Press, 121-151.
  • Lebra, S. T. (2004). The Japanese self in cultural logic. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i
    Press.
  • Lowie, R. H. (1923). Review of language. American Anthropologist,25: 90-93.
  • Lucy, J. A. (1992). Language diversity and thought. Cambridge: Cambridge University
    Press.
  • Lucy, J. A. (1997). Linguistic relativity. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26: 291-312.
  • Lucy, J. A. and Shweder, R. A. (1979). Whorf and his critics: linguistic and nonlinguistic
    influences on color memory. American Anthropologist, New Series, Vol. 81(3), 581-615.
  • Ohta, A. S. (1991). «Evidenciality and politeness in Japanese.» Issues in applied linguistics
    2(2), 211-238.
  • Ozgen, E., Davies, I. (2002). «Acquisition of categorical color perception: A perceptual
    learning approach to the linguistic relativity hypothesis.» Journal of Experimental Psychology,
    131(4), 477-493.
  • Pinker, S. (1995/2007). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. N.Y.:
    Harper Perennial.
  • Sapir, E. (1921/2004). Language: An introduction to the study of speech. N.Y.: Harcourt,
    Bruce & Company.
  • Sato, N. (2001/2007). Phenomenology of [seken] [Seken no genshoogaku
    (「世間」の現象学)]. Tokyo: Seikyusha.
  • Slobin, D. I. (1996). From «thought and language» to «thinking and speaking.» In:
    Rethinking Linguistic Relativity, John. J. Gumperz, and Steven. C. Levinson, Eds. Cambridge:
    Cambridge University Press. 70-96.
  • Slobin, D. I. (2003). Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic
    relativity.» In Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought, D. Gentner, and
    S. Goldin-Meadow, Eds. Cambridge, M.A.: MIT Press, 157-192.
  • Spier, L, Hallowell, A., Newman, S. (1941). Language, culture, and personality: Essays
    in memory of Edward Sapir. Menasha, Wis.: Sapir Memorial Publication Fund.
  • Tatara, N., Yagihashi, H. (2007). The interface of cognition and language − From
    linguistic relativity. [Ninchi to gengo no setten−Gengosoutaironwokangaeru] Journal of Human
    Linguistics, 6, 10-14.
  • Wardhaugh, R. (1986). An introduction to sociolinguistics. Malden, M.A.: Blackwell.
  • Wardy, R. (2006). Aristotle in China: Language, categories and translation. London:
    Cambridge University Press.
  • Wassmann, J., Dasen, P. (1998). Balinese spatial orientation: Some empirical evidence of
    moderate linguistic relativity. The Jounal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain
    and Ireland, 4(4), 689-711.
  • Watanabe, K. (2007). Discourse analysis of the first executive meeting for international
    merging: from the perspective of indexicity in addressing forms. [Kokusai gappeikigyo no
    daiikkaitorishimariyakukai no danwabunseki: Yobikakekeitai no shihyousei no shitenkara].
    Journal of Human Linguistics, 6, 15-18.
    30. Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, M.A.: MIT Press.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Author Biography

Downloads

Published

2014-12-09

Issue

Section

Vol 1 No 1 (2014)

How to Cite

Rieko Matsuoka. (2014). Does ‘Language’ Form Our ‘Thought’?. East European Journal of Psycholinguistics , 1(1), 138-145. https://eejpl.vnu.edu.ua/index.php/eejpl/article/view/221